The Brain and the Voice in Speech and Song eBook

This eBook from the Gutenberg Project consists of approximately 79 pages of information about The Brain and the Voice in Speech and Song.

The Brain and the Voice in Speech and Song eBook

This eBook from the Gutenberg Project consists of approximately 79 pages of information about The Brain and the Voice in Speech and Song.

The second quality of musical sounds is the pitch, and the pitch depends upon the number of vibrations that a sounding body makes in each second of time.  The most unmusical ear can distinguish a high note from a low one, even when the interval is not great.  Low notes are characterised by a relatively small number of vibrations, and as the pitch rises so the number of vibrations increase.  This can be proved in many ways.  Take, for example, two tuning-forks of different size:  the shorter produces a considerably higher pitched note than the longer one.  If a mirror be attached to one of the prongs of each fork, and a beam of light be cast upon each mirror successively and then reflected in a revolving mirror, the oscillating spot of light is converted into a series of waves; and if the waves obtained by reflecting the light from the mirror of the smaller one be counted and compared with those reflected from the mirror attached to the larger fork, it will be found that the number of waves reflected from the smaller fork is proportionally to the difference in the pitch more numerous than the waves reflected from the larger.  The air is thrown into corresponding periodic vibrations according to the rate of vibration of the sound-producing body.

Thirdly, the quality, timbre, or klang depends upon the overtones, in respect to which I could cite many experiments to prove that whenever a body vibrates, other bodies near it may be set in vibration, but only on condition that such bodies shall be capable themselves of producing the same note.  A number of different forms of resonators can be used to illustrate this law; a law indeed which is of the greatest importance in connection with the mechanism of the human voice.  Although notes are of the same loudness and pitch when played on different instruments or spoken or sung by different individuals, yet even a person with no ear for music can easily detect a difference in the quality of the sound and is able to recognise the nature of the instrument or the timbre of the voice.  This difference in the timbre is due to harmonics or overtones.  Could we but see the sonorous waves in the air during the transmission of the sound of a voice, we should see stamped on it the conditions of motion upon which its characteristic qualities depended; which is due to the fact that every vocal sound whose vibrations have a complex form can be decomposed into a series of simple notes all belonging to the harmonic series.  These harmonics or overtones will be considered later when dealing with the timbre or quality of the human voice.

The vocal instrument is unlike any other musical instrument; it most nearly approaches a reed instrument.  The clarionet and the oboe are examples of reed instruments, in which the reed does not alter but by means of stops the length of the column of air in the resonating pipe varies and determines the pitch of the fundamental note.  The organ-pipe with the vibrating tongue of metal serving as the reed is perhaps the nearest approach to the vocal organ; but here again it is the length of the pipe which determines the pitch of the note.

Copyrights
Project Gutenberg
The Brain and the Voice in Speech and Song from Project Gutenberg. Public domain.