The Brain and the Voice in Speech and Song eBook

This eBook from the Gutenberg Project consists of approximately 79 pages of information about The Brain and the Voice in Speech and Song.

The Brain and the Voice in Speech and Song eBook

This eBook from the Gutenberg Project consists of approximately 79 pages of information about The Brain and the Voice in Speech and Song.
the fact that there are no conscious kinaesthetic impressions of alterations of position and tension of the vocal cords.  A comparative microscopic examination of the tip of the tongue and the lips shows a remarkable difference, for these structures are beset with innumerable sensory nerves, whereby every slightest alteration of tension and minute variations in degrees of pressure of the covering skin is associated with messages thereon to the brain.  The sense of movement in articulate speech is therefore explained by this fact.  There is every reason then to believe that auditory tonal images are the sole primary and essential guides to the minute alterations of tension in the muscles of the larynx necessary for the production of corresponding vocal sounds.  By humming a tune we concentrate our attention and thereby limit the activity of neural processes to systems and communities of neurones employed for the perception of tonal images and their activation in motor processes; and this helps to fix the tune in the memory.

PSYCHIC MECHANISM OF THE VOICE

A musical speaking voice denotes generally a good singing voice, and it must be remembered that articulation cannot be separated from phonation in the psychic mechanism.  In speaking, we are unconscious of the breath necessary for the production of the voice.  Not so, however, in effective singing, the management of the breathing being of fundamental importance; and it is no exaggeration to say that only the individual who knows how to breathe knows how to sing effectually.  A musical ear and sense of rhythm are innate in some individuals; in others they are not innate and can only be acquired to a variable degree of perfection by persevering efforts and practice.  The most intelligent persons may never be able to sing in tune, or even time; the latter (sense of rhythm) is much more easily acquired by practice than the former (correct intonation).  This is easily intelligible, for rhythmical movement appertains also to speech and other acts of human beings, e.g. walking, dancing, running, swimming, etc.; moreover, rhythmical periodicity characterises the beat of the heart and respiration.

But how does a trained singer learn to sing a song or to take part in an opera?  He has to study the performances of two parts for the vocal instrument—­the part written by the composer and the part written by the poet or dramatist—­and in order to present an artistic rendering, the intellectual and emotional characters of each part must be blended in harmonious combination.  A singer will first read the words and understand their meaning, then memorise them, so that the whole attention subsequently may be given to applying the musical part to them and employing with proper phrasing, which means more than knowing when to breathe; it means imparting expression and feeling.  A clever actor or orator can, if he possess a high degree of intelligence and a fairly artistic

Copyrights
Project Gutenberg
The Brain and the Voice in Speech and Song from Project Gutenberg. Public domain.